• the post of tom joad
    link
    fedilink
    arrow-up
    8
    ·
    edit-2
    5 months ago

    This correlation seems to have nothing to do with blast exposure as the title suggests From the article

    The rates for these combat occupations are roughly twice those of service members who work in noncombat jobs like data processing or food service.

    The article even mentions there isn’t any implied causation between blasts and non-blast exposed combat troops either in the data presented (except maybe a 4% difference in suicide rate between artillery crews and combat troops with less exposure). The data they are presenting* shows the largest drop in rate is between combat and non combat troops How can the author have this information and come away with this conclusion?

    • Hegar@fedia.io
      link
      fedilink
      arrow-up
      13
      ·
      edit-2
      5 months ago

      The paragraph before the one you quoted says it:

      Explosives ordinance disposal team members, who disable roadside bombs and routinely train and work around very large blasts, had the highest suicide rate — 34.77 deaths per 100,000 people per year — followed by infantry and special operations forces; armor crews; and artillery troops; whose rates are closer to 30 deaths per 100,000.

      That’s a 15% bump in suicide for the most blast exposed troops when compared to less blast exposed combat troops.

      Obviously combat exposure vs not is going to have a large effect, but blast exposure seems to have a further effect on top the horrors of combat.

      Also:

      In the Air Force, where blast exposure is rare, there were no significant differences in suicide rates among different military occupations. But among Army and Marine Corps troops, the rates are elevated wherever blasts are part of daily work

      The report presented the data, but made no attempt to grapple with the implications:

      The report released on Wednesday does not mention blast exposure as a factor, and offers no insights into what may be contributing to the different suicide rates. Still, the correlation between deaths by suicide and levels of blast exposure is a common theme in the figures

      • the post of tom joad
        link
        fedilink
        arrow-up
        6
        arrow-down
        1
        ·
        edit-2
        5 months ago

        Alright, now here’s where i am ready to be wrong… How is an increasing suicide rate from 30 per 100k to 35 per 100k statistically significant? Am i even worse at math than i thought?

        • Hegar@fedia.io
          link
          fedilink
          arrow-up
          9
          ·
          edit-2
          5 months ago

          It’s the same reason that an increase from a 0.0001% chance of something happening to a 0.0002% chance is a 100% increased chance of the thing happening.

          More relevantly, 0.0003477% is ~ 15% bigger a number than 0.0003000%. The overall numbers are still low, but the increase is significant.

          • the post of tom joad
            link
            fedilink
            arrow-up
            8
            ·
            edit-2
            5 months ago

            Well then i guess i am wrong, cuz i have to concede 15% is a significant difference. Thanks for explaining that. It still feels like artificial inflation of stats to my mind, but that’s just like, my opinion, man

              • rekorse@lemmy.world
                link
                fedilink
                arrow-up
                3
                ·
                5 months ago

                I remember reading over some logic - statistics “puzzles”, where the logic is sound if you follow step by step on paper, but for some reason your mind just has trouble grasping the concept which leads to confusion.

    • Aurix@lemmy.world
      link
      fedilink
      arrow-up
      10
      arrow-down
      1
      ·
      5 months ago

      The New York Times, that’s why. It’s a paper which drastically lost credibility.